Universal law of generalization![]() The universal law of generalization is a theory of cognition stating that the probability of a response to one stimulus being generalized to another is a function of the “distance” between the two stimuli in a psychological space. It was introduced in 1987 by Roger N. Shepard,[1][2] who began researching mechanisms of generalization while he was still a graduate student at Yale:
Shepard’s 1987 paper gives a "generalization" example of a bird that has eaten one earthworm, and is presented with a slightly different-looking earthworm.[2] Explaining the concept of "psychological space" in the abstract of his 1987 paper, Shepard wrote:
Using experimental evidence from both human and non-human subjects, Shepard hypothesized, more specifically, that the probability of generalization will fall off exponentially with the distance measured by one of two particular metrics. His analysis goes on to argue for the universality of this rule for all sentient organisms, due to evolutionary internalization. Additional Research and CommentaryIn 1988, Shepard received a response to his research from Daniel M. Ennis from the Philip Morris Research Center.[4] Ennis called into question whether Shepard’s theory was relevant as it echoed research already accomplished by Nosofsky, whose research presented “several exceptions” [4] to Shepard’s theory. Shepard responded to him by asserting that Nosofsky’s experiments focused on “ the precise locations of individual stimuli in ‘psychological space’” [4] whereas his experiments focused on the “location, size, and shape of the region of psychological space corresponding to the set of stimuli having the same important consequence as a given training stimulus”.[4] Other researchers have followed up Shepard’s research with their own looks into the law of generalization. In 2000, Ken Cheng from Macquarie University [5] experimented on the special generalization of honeybees, comparing his findings with earlier research on humans and pigeons. Cheng explained his understanding of Shepard’s law of generalization in this study as such:
Cheng measured the bee’s response to copies of a container in different locations, the original staying in one area and holding sugar water. The copy containers would then either have plain tap water or sugar water. Measurements of generalization gradients were separated by both distance and direction according to Shepard’s law. Cheng found that “generalization gradients in both series were fitted by exponential functions, supporting Shepard’s law”.[5] Cheng’s research suggested that Shepard’s law had the potential to be generalized to more than only mammals and birds but to invertebrates as well. In 2001, Chater and Vitányi [6] attempted to provide a “mathematically more appealing form of the Universal Law”.[6] They explain the necessity of a different mathematical outlook than the one presented in Shepard’s paper by illustrating the example of a photograph and its negative:
Chater and Vitányi admit that their way of approaching the law of generalization might be too abstract to fit a psychological equation, but argue that a simple abstract explanation would be as suitable as any other elementary explanation in a practical sense when faced with situations that are webbed with complex mathematical explanations.[6] Chris R. Sims [7] has attempted to offer another look at the law of generalization through the scope of the principle of efficient coding. Sims makes the case for rate-distortion theory through perceptual identification experiments.[7] Steven A. Frank of the University of California, Irvine [8] proposed another outlook on the approach of the law of generalization. He argues that the exponential form of the universal law arises simply because it is the only mathematical transformation from a continuous perceptual scale to a response probability which is invariant to shift and stretch. References
|
Portal di Ensiklopedia Dunia