N170

The N170 is a component of the event-related potential (ERP) that reflects the neural processing of faces, familiar objects or words.[1] Furthermore, the N170 is modulated by prediction error processes.[2][3]

When potentials evoked by images of faces are compared to those elicited by other visual stimuli, the former show increased negativity 130-200 ms after stimulus presentation. This response is maximal over occipito-temporal electrode sites, which is consistent with a source located at the fusiform and inferior-temporal gyri, confirmed by electrocorticography.[4][5] The N170 generally displays right-hemisphere lateralization and has been linked with the structural encoding of faces, hence is considered to be primarily sensitive to faces.[6][7] A study, employing transcranial magnetic stimulation combined with EEG, found that N170 can be modulated by top-down influences from prefrontal cortex.[8]

History

The N170 was first described by Shlomo Bentin and colleagues in 1996,[9] who measured ERPs from participants viewing faces and other objects. They found that human faces and face parts (such as eyes) elicited different responses than other stimuli, including animal faces, body parts, and cars.

Earlier work performed by Botzel and Grusser and first reported in 1989[10] also attempted to find a component of the ERP that corresponded to the processing of human faces. They showed observers line drawings (in one experiment) and black-and-white photographs (in two additional experiments) of faces, trees, and chairs. They found that, compared to the other stimulus classes, faces elicited a larger positive component approximately 150 ms after onset, which was maximal at central electrode sites (at the top of the head). The topography of this effect and lack of lateralization led to the conclusion that this face-specific potential did not arise in face-selective areas in the occipital-temporal region, but instead in the limbic system. Subsequent work referred to this component as the vertex positive potential (VPP).[11]

In an attempt to rectify these two apparently conflicting results, Joyce and Rossion[12] recorded ERPs from 53 scalp electrodes while participants viewed faces and other visual stimuli. After recording, they re-referenced the data to several commonly used reference electrode sites, including the nose and mastoid process. They found that the N170 and VPP can be accounted for by the same dipole arrangement arising from the same neural generators, and therefore reflect the same process.

Functional sensitivity

Three of the most studied attributes of the N170 include manipulations of face inversion, facial race, and emotional expressions.

It has been established that inverted faces (i.e., those presented upside-down) are more difficult to perceive[13] (the Thatcher effect is a good illustration of this). In their landmark study, Bentin et al. found that inverted faces increased the latency of the N170 component.[9] Jacques and colleagues further studied the timecourse of the face inversion effect (FIE) using an adaptation paradigm.[14] When the same stimulus is presented multiple times, the neuronal response decreases over time; when a different stimulus is presented, the response recovers. The conditions under which a "release from adaptation" occurs therefore provides a way to measure stimulus similarity. In their experiment, Jacques et al. found that the release from adaptation is smaller and occurs 30 ms later for inverted faces, indicating that the neuronal population encoding face identity require additional processing time to detect the identity of inverted faces.

In an experiment examining the effects of race on the N170's amplitude, it was found that an "Other-Race Effect" was elicited in conjunction with face inversions. Vizioli and colleagues examined the effect of face recognition impairment while subjects process same race (SR) or other race (OR) pictures.[15] The research team devised a N170 experiment based on the premise that visual expertise plays a critical role in inversion, hypothesizing that viewers' greater level of expertise with SR faces (holistic processing) should elicit a stronger FIE compared to OR face stimuli. The authors recorded EEGs from Western Caucasian and East Asian subjects (two separate groups) who were presented with pictures of Western Caucasian, East Asian and African America faces in upright and inverted orientations. All the facial stimuli were cropped to remove external features (i.e. hair, beards, hats, etc.). Both groups displayed a later N170 with larger amplitude (over the right hemisphere) for inverted than upright same-race (SR) faces, but showed no inversion effect for OR and AA photo stimuli. Moreover, no race effects were observed in regard to the peak amplitude of the N170 for upright faces in both groups of participants. The results also found no significant latency differences among the races of stimuli, but facial inversion did increase and delay the N170 amplitude and onset respectively. They conclude that the subjects' lack of experience with inverted faces makes processing such stimuli more difficult than pictures shown in their canonical orientation, regardless of what race the stimulus is.

Besides modulation by inversion and race, emotional expressions have also been a focus of N170 face research. In an experiment conducted by Righart and de Gelder, ERP results show that the early stages of face processing may be affected by emotional scenes when categorizations of fearful and happy facial expressions are made by subjects.[16] In this paradigm subjects had to view color pictures of happy or fearful faces that were centrally overlaid on pictures of natural scenes. And in order to control for low level features, such as color and other items that could care meaning, all the scene pictures were scrambled by randomizing the position of pixels across the image. The final results of the experiment show that emotion effects were associated with the N170 in which there was a larger (negative) amplitude for faces when they appeared in a fearful context then when placed in happy or neutral scenes. In fact, left occipito-temporal distributed N170 amplitudes were dramatically increased for intact fearful faces when they appeared in a fearful scene, though levels were not as high when a fearful face was presented in a happy or neutral scene. Similar results did occur in regard to intact happy faces, but the amplitudes were not as high as those related to fearful scenes or expressions.[17] Righart and de Gelder conclude that information from task-irrelevant scenes is rapidly combined with the information from facial expressions, and that subjects use context information in the early stage of processing when they need to discriminate/categorize facial expressions.

Results from a study conducted by Ghuman and colleagues using direct neural recordings from the fusiform face area using electrocorticography showed that while the N170 displays a very strong response to faces when compared to other visual images, the N170 is not sensitive to the identity of the face.[4] Instead, they showed that which face a person is viewing can be decoded from the activity between 250–500 ms, consistent with the hypothesis that identity processing begins with the N250.[18] These results suggest that the N170 is important for gist-level processing of faces and face detection, processes which may set the stage for later face individuation.

Generators

Given the ease and rapidity with which humans can recognize faces, a great deal of neuroscientific research has endeavored to understand how and where the brain processes them. Early research on prosopagnosia, or "face blindness", found that damage to the occipito-temporal region led to an impaired or complete inability for people to recognize faces. Convergent evidence for the importance of this region in face processing came through the use of fMRI, which found that a region of the fusiform gyrus, the "fusiform face area", responded selectively to images of faces.

Intracranial recordings in humans using electrocorticography provide very strong evidence that the fusiform face area is one of the generators of the N170,[4][5] though other regions of the face processing network may also contribute to the N170.

An investigation of the N170 undertaken[19] used ERP source-localization techniques to estimate the location of the neural generator of the N170. They concluded that the N170 arose from the posterior superior temporal sulcus. However, these techniques are fraught with potential sources of error, and there is disagreement on the validity of inferences drawn from such findings.[20]

Faces or interstimulus variance

In 2007, Guillaume Thierry and colleagues[21] presented evidence that called into question the face-specificity of the N170. Most earlier experiments found an N170 when the response to frontal views of faces was compared to those of other objects that could appear in more variable poses and configurations. In their study, they introduced a new factor: stimuli could be faces or non-faces, and either class could have high or low similarity. Similarity was measured by calculating the correlation between pixel values in pairs of same-category stimuli. When ERPs were compared for these conditions, they found a typical N170 effect in the low-similarity non-face vs. high-similarity face comparison. However, high-similarity non-faces showed a significant N170, while low-similarity faces did not. These results led the authors to conclude that the N170 is actually a measure of stimulus similarity, and not face processing per se.

In response to this, Rossion and Jacques[7] measured similarity as above for several object categories used in a previous study of the N170. They found that faces elicited a larger N170 than other classes of objects that had similar or higher similarity values, such as houses, cars, and shoes. While it remains uncertain why Thierry et al. observed an effect of similarity on the N170, Rossion and Jacques speculate that lower similarity leads to more variance in the latency of the response. Since ERP components are measured by averaging the results from many individual trials, high latency variance effectively “smears” the response, reducing the amplitude of the average. Rossion and Jacques also offer criticism of the methodology used by Thierry and colleagues, arguing that their failure to find a difference between high-similarity faces and high-similarity non-faces was due to a poor choice of electrode sites.

See also

References

  1. ^ Rossion, Bruno; Joyce, Carrie A; Cottrell, Garrison W; Tarr, Michael J (November 2003). "Early lateralization and orientation tuning for face, word, and object processing in the visual cortex". NeuroImage. 20 (3): 1609–1624. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.010. PMID 14642472. S2CID 6844697.
  2. ^ Johnston, Patrick; Robinson, Jonathan; Kokkinakis, Athanasios; Ridgeway, Samuel; Simpson, Michael; Johnson, Sam; Kaufman, Jordy; Young, Andrew W. (April 2017). "Temporal and spatial localization of prediction-error signals in the visual brain" (PDF). Biological Psychology. 125: 45–57. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.02.004. PMID 28257807. S2CID 10308172.
  3. ^ Robinson, Jonathan E.; Breakspear, Michael; Young, Andrew W.; Johnston, Patrick J. (16 April 2018). "Dose-dependent modulation of the visually evoked N1/N170 by perceptual surprise: a clear demonstration of prediction-error signalling" (PDF). European Journal of Neuroscience. 52 (11): 4442–4452. doi:10.1111/ejn.13920. PMID 29602233. S2CID 4887440.
  4. ^ a b c Ghuman, Avniel Singh; Brunet, Nicolas M.; Li, Yuanning; Konecky, Roma O.; Pyles, John A.; Walls, Shawn A.; Destefino, Vincent; Wang, Wei; Richardson, R. Mark (2014-01-01). "Dynamic encoding of face information in the human fusiform gyrus". Nature Communications. 5: 5672. Bibcode:2014NatCo...5.5672G. doi:10.1038/ncomms6672. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 4339092. PMID 25482825.
  5. ^ a b Allison, T.; Puce, A.; Spencer, D. D.; McCarthy, G. (1999-08-01). "Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli". Cerebral Cortex. 9 (5): 415–430. doi:10.1093/cercor/9.5.415. ISSN 1047-3211. PMID 10450888.
  6. ^ Eimer, Martin (2011). "The Face-Sensitive N170 Component of the Event-Related Brain Potential". In Calder, Andrew J.; Rhodes, Gillian; Johnson, Mark H.; Haxby, James V. (eds.). Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. OUP Oxford. ISBN 9780191622144.
  7. ^ a b Rossion, B. & Jacques, C. (2008). "Does physical interstimulus variance account for early electrophysiological face sensitive responses in the human brain? Ten lessons on the N170". NeuroImage. 39 (4): 1959–1979. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.011. PMID 18055223. S2CID 15106925.
  8. ^ Mattavelli, G.; Rosanova, M.; Casali, A. G.; Papagno, C.; Romero Lauro L. J. (2013). "Top-down interference and cortical responsiveness in face processing: A TMS-EEG study". NeuroImage. 76 (1): 24–32. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.020. PMID 23523809. S2CID 13911132.
  9. ^ a b Bentin, S.; McCarthy, G.; Perez, E.; Puce, A.; Allison, T. (1996). "Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 8 (6): 551–565. doi:10.1162/jocn.1996.8.6.551. PMC 2927138. PMID 20740065.
  10. ^ Botzel, K.; Grusser, O. J. (1989). "Electric brain potentials-evoked by pictures of faces and non-faces – a search for face-specific EEG-potentials". Experimental Brain Research. 77 (2): 349–360. doi:10.1007/BF00274992. PMID 2792281. S2CID 5373013.
  11. ^ Jeffreys, D. A. (1989). "A face-responsive potential recorded from the human scalp". Experimental Brain Research. 78 (1): 193–202. doi:10.1007/BF00230699. PMID 2591512. S2CID 12084552.
  12. ^ Joyce, C.A. & Rossion, B. (2005). "The face-sensitive N170 and VPP components manifest the same brain processes: The effect of reference electrode site". Clinical Neurophysiology. 116 (11): 2613–2631. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2005.07.005. PMID 16214404. S2CID 34009900.
  13. ^ Yin R. K. (1969). "Looking at upside-down faces". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 81 (1): 141–145. doi:10.1037/h0027474.
  14. ^ Jacques, C.; d'Arripe, O.; Rossion, B. (2007). "The time course of the face inversion effect during individual face discrimination". Journal of Vision. 7 (3): 1–9. doi:10.1167/7.8.3. PMID 17685810.
  15. ^ Vizioli, L.; Foreman, K.; Rousselet, G. A.; Caldara, R. (2010). "Inverting faces elicits sensitivity to race on the N170 component: a cross-cultural study". Journal of Vision. 10 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1167/10.1.15. PMID 20143908.
  16. ^ Righart, R. & de Gelder, B. (2008). "Rapid influence of emotional scenes on encoding of facial expressions: an ERP study. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 3 (3): 270–8. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn021. PMC 2566764. PMID 19015119.
  17. ^ Blau, V. C.; Maurer, U.; Tottenham, N.; McCandliss, B. D. (2007). "The face-specific N170 component is modulated by emotional facial expression". Behavioral and Brain Functions. 3: 7. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-3-7. PMC 1794418. PMID 17244356.
  18. ^ Tanaka, James W.; Curran, Tim; Porterfield, Albert L.; Collins, Daniel (2006-09-01). "Activation of preexisting and acquired face representations: the N250 event-related potential as an index of face familiarity". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 18 (9): 1488–1497. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.543.8563. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1488. ISSN 0898-929X. PMID 16989550. S2CID 9793157.
  19. ^ Itier, R. J.; Taylor, M. J. (2004). "Source analysis of the N170 to faces and objects". NeuroReport. 15 (8): 1261–1265. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000127827.73576.d8. PMID 15167545. S2CID 46705488.
  20. ^ Luck, S. J. (2005). "ERP Localization". An Introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique. Boston: MIT Press. pp. 267–301.
  21. ^ Thierry, G.; Martin, C. D.; Downing, P.; Pegna, A. J. (2007). "Controlling for interstimulus perceptual variance abolishes N170 face selectivity". Nature Neuroscience. 10 (7): 505–511. doi:10.1038/nn1864. PMID 17334361. S2CID 21008862.
  • Bruno Rossion's lab has an overview of their research on the N170.

Read other articles:

United States historic placeMeridian HighwayU.S. National Register of Historic Places Bridge on the Meridian Highway in Pierce County, Nebraska; seen from the southwestNearest cityPierce, NebraskaArea36 acres (15 ha)Built1911 (1911)NRHP reference No.01001273[1]Added to NRHPNovember 29, 2001 Meridian Highway was a United States auto trail in the early twentieth century. It roughly followed the path of present-day U.S. Route 81 from Pembina, North Dakota to Fort Worth, …

Fagales Fagus sylvatica Klasifikasi ilmiah Kerajaan: Plantae (tanpa takson): Tracheophyta (tanpa takson): Angiospermae (tanpa takson): Eudikotil (tanpa takson): Rosid (tanpa takson): Fabid Ordo: Fagales Famili lihat teks. Fagales adalah salah satu ordo tumbuhan berbunga yang termasuk dalam klad euRosidae I, Rosidae, core Eudikotil, dan Eudikotil (Sistem klasifikasi APG II). Bangsa ini juga diakui sebagai takson dalam sistem klasifikasi Cronquist dan tercakup dalam anak kelas Hamamelidae, kelas M…

Video game series by Ninja Kiwi For the first game in the series, see § Bloons Tower Defense. Video game seriesBloons Tower DefenseThe logo of Bloons Tower Defense, the first game in the series.Genre(s)Tower defenseDeveloper(s) Ninja Kiwi Digital Goldfish Ltd. (iOS) Publisher(s)Ninja KiwiPlatform(s)Android, browser (Flash), iOS, macOS, Nintendo DSi, PlayStation Portable, Microsoft Windows, Xbox One, Nintendo SwitchFirst releaseBloons TDAugust 16, 2007Latest releaseBloons TD Battles 2Novemb…

Wali Kota BaubauPetahanaMuhammad Rasman ManafiPenjabatsejak 25 September 2023Pemerintah Kota BaubauKediamanRumah Jabatan Wali Kota BaubauMasa jabatan5 tahun dan dapat dipilih kembali untuk satu kali masa jabatanDibentuk21 Juni 2001; 22 tahun lalu (2001-06-21)Pejabat pertamaBelum diketahuiSitus webSitus web resmi Berikut adalah daftar wali kota Baubau dari masa ke masa. No. Wali Kota Potret Partai Awal Akhir Masa jabatan Periode Wakil Ref. 1 Mz. Amirul Tamim(lahir 1954) PPP 2003 2008 4…

Une nouvelle amie Données clés Réalisation François Ozon Scénario François Ozond'après la nouvelle de Ruth Rendell Musique Philippe Rombi Acteurs principaux Romain DurisAnaïs DemoustierRaphaël Personnaz Sociétés de production Mandarin Cinéma Pays de production France Genre Film dramatique Durée 105 minutes Sortie 2014 Pour plus de détails, voir Fiche technique et Distribution. modifier Une nouvelle amie est un mélodrame français écrit et réalisé par François Ozon, sorti en 20…

Nuclear-powered cruise missile 9M730 Burevestnik TypeNuclear-powered cruise missilePlace of originRussiaService historyIn serviceUnder developmentSpecificationsLength12 m[1]Effective firing rangeEffectively unlimitedWarheadThermonuclear Satellite imagery of the launch site The 9M730 Burevestnik (Russian: Буревестник; Storm petrel, NATO reporting name: SSC-X-9 Skyfall)[2][3][4] is a Russian low-flying, nuclear-powered, nuclear-ar…

Indo-Aryan language primarily spoken in Gilgit-Baltistan and Gurez valley Not to be confused with Kohistani Shina. Shinaݜݨیاٗ زبان / ݜݨیاٗ گلیتوࣿ زبانṢiṇyaáThe word Ṣiṇyaá written in the Arabic script in Nastaliq style.Pronunciation[ʂiɳjá]Native toPakistan, IndiaRegionGilgit-Baltistan, Kohistan, Drass, GurezEthnicityShinaNative speakers720,200 Shina (2018)[1]and Shina, Kohistani 458,000 (2018)[2]Language familyIndo-European …

Intense dislike or fear of Eastern Orthodoxy, hostility or prejudice towards Orthodox Christians This article is about negative attitudes towards Eastern Orthodox Christians and acts which have been committed against them because of their faith. For Anti-Orthodox theological positions and various theoretical and philosophical disputes during Christian history, see History of Christian theology. Freedom of religion Concepts Laicism Religious discrimination Religious censorship Religious liberty R…

Parliamentary constituency in the United Kingdom, 1950–1974 Leicester North WestFormer Borough constituencyfor the House of Commons1950–February 1974SeatsoneCreated fromLeicester WestReplaced byLeicester West Leicester North West was a borough constituency in the city of Leicester. It returned one Member of Parliament (MP) to the House of Commons of the Parliament of the United Kingdom. The constituency was created for the 1950 general election, and abolished for the February 1974 general el…

亞該亞戰爭《科林斯的最後一天》,由東尼·羅伯特-弗勒里(英语:Tony Robert-Fleury)於1870年所繪日期前146年地点希臘本土结果 羅馬獲得勝利领土变更 羅馬佔領希臘本土参战方 羅馬共和國 亞該亞同盟指挥官与领导者 昆圖斯·凱基利烏斯·梅特盧斯盧基烏斯·穆米烏斯 梅格洛玻利斯的克里圖勞斯迪亞厄斯 亞該亞戰爭,是前146年羅馬共和國與希臘亞該亞同盟之間所爆發的戰爭,亞…

Deaf sign language of Cuba Cuban Sign LanguageFingerspelling of LSCNative toCubaSigners34,000 (2021)[1]Language family(unclassified)[2]Language codesISO 639-3csfGlottologcuba1235 LSC Dactilema Cuban Sign Language, (Spanish: Lengua de señas cubana, LSC) is the language used by the Deaf community in Cuba. There are approximately 19,000 users of the language.[3] Cuban Sign Language is an important part of the culture of the Deaf community in Cuba.[4] Histo…

Old and stable part of the continental lithosphere Cratons of South America and Africa during the Triassic Period when the two continents were joined as part of the Pangea supercontinent A craton ( /ˈkreɪtɒn/ KRAYT-on, /ˈkrætɒn/ KRAT-on, or /ˈkreɪtən/ KRAY-tən;[1][2][3] from Greek: κράτος kratos strength) is an old and stable part of the continental lithosphere, which consists of Earth's two topmost layers, the crust and the uppermost mantle. Having often …

يفتقر محتوى هذه المقالة إلى الاستشهاد بمصادر. فضلاً، ساهم في تطوير هذه المقالة من خلال إضافة مصادر موثوق بها. أي معلومات غير موثقة يمكن التشكيك بها وإزالتها. (يوليو 2017) حريما الإحداثيات 32°38′17.4″N 35°50′17.6″E / 32.638167°N 35.838222°E / 32.638167; 35.838222 تقسيم إداري  البلد الأردن&#…

1288 treaty between England and Aragon The Treaty of Canfranc was an agreement, signed in October 1288, between Edward I of England and Alfonso III of Aragon about the release of Charles II of Naples,[1] who had been captured by the Admiral of Sicily, Roger of Lauria, in a naval battle on 5 June 1284.[2][3] References ^ Bárány 2010, p. 71. ^ Bárány 2010, p. 68. ^ Runciman 1958, p. 246. Sources Bárány, Attila (2010). The English relations of Charles II…

Tenis padaPekan Olahraga Nasional XIX Tunggal putra putri Ganda putra putri campuran Beregu putra putri Pertandingan tenis tunggal putra pada Pekan Olahraga Nasional XIX berlangsung di Lapang Tenis Siliwangi dan Taman Maluku, Kompleks olahraga Siliwangi, Kota Bandung, Jawa Barat dari tanggal 23 sampai 27 september 2016.[1] Jadwal Tanggal 22−23 Sep 23−24 Sep 25 Sep 26 Sep 27 Sep Tunggal putra Babak 32 besar Babak 16 besar Perempatfinal Semifinal Final Unggulan Arief Rahman (KTM) (Fina…

Letkol Tek (Purn.)Basir Surya Informasi pribadiLahir(1914-07-17)17 Juli 1914Garut, Jawa Barat, Hindia BelandaMeninggal20 Desember 1993(1993-12-20) (umur 79)Tasikmalaya, Jawa BaratKarier militerPihak IndonesiaDinas/cabang TNI Angkatan UdaraPangkat LetkolSatuanKorps TeknikSunting kotak info • L • B Letnan Kolonel Tek. (Purn.) Basir Surya (17 Juli 1914 – 20 Desember 1993) adalah seorang perwira Teknik TNI Angkatan Udara, komandan pertama Lanud Wiriadinata tah…

Armando MadonnaMadonna all'Atalanta nel 1988Nazionalità Italia Altezza180 cm Peso74 kg Calcio RuoloAllenatore (ex centrocampista, attaccante) Termine carriera2002 - giocatore CarrieraGiovanili 1980-1981 Atalanta Squadre di club1 1981-1983 Atalanta19 (1)1983-1988 Piacenza169 (43)1988-1990 Atalanta57 (12)1990-1991 Lazio25 (2)1991-1992→  Piacenza27 (5)1992-1993 SPAL21 (0)1993-2002 Alzano Virescit259 (29) Carriera da allenatore 2002-2003 Alzano Vi…

此條目没有列出任何参考或来源。 (2018年3月3日)維基百科所有的內容都應該可供查證。请协助補充可靠来源以改善这篇条目。无法查证的內容可能會因為異議提出而被移除。 河合曾良出生1649年??月??日 日本 信濃国下桑原村逝世1710年6月18日 日本 壱岐国勝本職業俳諧師體裁俳句代表作曾良旅日記 河合曾良(日语:河合 曾良/かわい そら Kawai Sora,1649年—1710年6月18…

Quella strana ragazza che abita in fondo al vialeJodie Foster in una scena del filmTitolo originaleThe Little Girl Who Lives Down the Lane Paese di produzioneCanada, Francia Anno1976 Durata94 min e 96 min Rapporto16/9 Generedrammatico, thriller RegiaNicolas Gessner SoggettoLaird Koenig (racconto) SceneggiaturaLaird Koenig ProduttoreZev Braung FotografiaRené Verzier MontaggioYves Langlois MusicheChristian Gaubert Interpreti e personaggi Jodie Foster: Rynn Jacobs Martin Sheen: Frank Halle…

Pour les articles homonymes, voir Dimanche (homonymie) et Jour du Seigneur. Un dimanche, par Paul Signac, 1890. Le dimanche est le jour de la semaine privilégié pour le repos hebdomadaire dans plusieurs pays du monde, dont la France. La norme internationale ISO 8601 considère que le dimanche clôt la semaine[1], et le code avec le chiffre 7. Pour les religions bibliques, le dimanche est le premier jour de la semaine juive et chrétienne. Pour les chrétiens, le dimanche est le jour de la rés…