Emotional responsivity

Emotional responsivity is the ability to acknowledge an affective stimuli by exhibiting emotion.[1] It is a sharp change of emotion according to a person's emotional state.[2] Increased emotional responsivity refers to demonstrating more response to a stimulus. Reduced emotional responsivity refers to demonstrating less response to a stimulus.[3] Any response exhibited after exposure to the stimulus, whether it is appropriate or not, would be considered as an emotional response. Although emotional responsivity applies to nonclinical populations, it is more typically associated with individuals with schizophrenia and autism.

Emotional responsivity is connected to broader psychology concepts about emotions. People exhibit emotions in response to outside stimuli. Positive affective stimuli trigger feelings of pleasure such as happiness; negative affective stimuli trigger feelings of displeasure such as disgust and fear.[3] Emotional responses include but are not limited to facial expressions and neurophysiological activities. For example, people display a “smile” when exposed to positive stimuli and a “frown” when exposed to negative stimuli. The feeling associated with emotion is called an affect, which can be categorized by valence and arousal. Valence describes the degree to which the feeling is a pleasure or displeasure. Arousal describes the degree to which a person is awoken by outside stimuli.[4] 

Experimental measures

Clinical studies of emotional responsivity involve two essential procedures. First, the researchers try to stimulate emotions from the participants by engaging participants in specific tasks. Then, the researchers measure the degree to which the participants respond to the stimuli.

Tasks used to stimulate emotional responses include:

  • Picture stimuli: this task involves showing participants pleasant or unpleasant pictures and ask participants to rate the arousal and valence level of the picture. A pleasant picture could depict opposite-sex erotica; an unpleasant picture could be a burned human body.[5] The International Affective Picture System is a photo database that generates pictures designed to arouse specific emotions.[4]
  • Film clips: this task asks participants to view film clips that arouse different emotions. For example, comedies’ clips trigger potential feelings of joy. Neutral clips such as scenes of rainforests usually are included to serve as a control.[6]
  • Experimenter distress: this task involves the experimenter to stimulate emotions from participants by acting scenarios of distress. For example, the experimenter bans his or her knee and display expressions of pain.[7]
  • Facial stimuli: this task involves asking a participant to view pictures of human faces that display different facial expressions. Participants would then be asked to rate their emotional experience. The facial expression would be considered as the affective stimuli.
  • Yummy-yucky: in this task, a child is asked to eat a certain food that was chosen by his or her parent. An experimenter then tastes the chosen food and displays either pleased or disgusted facial expression.[8]
  • Surprise box: this task intends to stimulate an emotional response by opening a box in front of a participant, usually a child, and displaying either an “amazed” reaction by saying “ohh” or a “frightened” reaction by saying “ahh”.[9]

After exposing participants to affective stimuli, researchers typically use the following methods to measure, record, and sometimes code emotional responsivity:

  • Skin conductance (electrodermal activity): Skin conductivity indicates emotional arousal because increased sweat production by certain emotions increases the skin conductivity level.[4]
  • fMRI: this technology generates neuro-images of brain activities. fMRI can detect neurophysiological changes in the brain and thereby help determine the degree to which a person is aroused by the stimulus.[5] For example, increased amygdala activity can be an indication of fear.[10]
  • Facial Action Coding System (FACS): this method uses the taxonomy of the face to measure movements of facial muscles. For example, the contraction of the orbicularis oculi muscle is an indication of happiness.[6]
  • Maximally Discriminative Facial Movement Coding System: this system is mostly used to measure the emotional responsivity of young children by corresponding universally recognized facial expressions to emotions. For example, lowering eyebrows indicates emotions of anger.[11]
  • Self-report: this method asks the participants to rate their valence and arousal level through questionnaires such as the Emotionally Expressivity Scales and the Berkley Expressivity Questionnaire.[3] Studies have shown that self-report might not correspond to objective measures such as fMRI.[10]
  • Ambulatory Blood Pressure Monitoring (ABPM): Tool used to measure blood pressure. Increases in blood pressure show remarkable levels of emotional responsivity.[2]

Emotional responsivity in mental illness

Autism is associated with decreased emotional responsivity. There was a study involving twenty-six children with autism and fifteen children with other learning disabilities, in which an adult displayed some form of emotion to study how the children respond. They focused on attention, hedonic tone, latency to changes in tone and an emotional contagion summery was made. Studies show correlations between measures of joint attention, emotional contagion, and the severity of autism. Results demonstrate that children with autism did not demonstrate changes in affect; however, their responses occurred much less than in comparison groups.[9]

In another study involving twenty-one autism patients, FACS analysis demonstrates that people with autism display less facial responsivity when watching evocative films. Specifically, when compared to the control group, the autism group does not demonstrate the more complex muscular movements and displayed less differentiated facial responses when exposed to stimuli. This study confirms that autism impedes social interaction and cognition.[6]

Schizophrenia impacts emotional responsivity by reducing a person’s hedonic capacity and producing a blunted affect. Patients usually have an increased emotional response to displeasure and a decreased emotional response to pleasure. A study involving 22 outpatients demonstrates that schizophrenia increases the emotional responsivity to low arousing negative stimuli while decreases the emotional responsivity to high arousing positive stimuli. People with Schizophrenia exhibit fewer facial expressions when watching evocative films.[12]

There are differences in the arousal level of stimuli between paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenia. Non-paranoid patients have increased negative emotional responsitivity and decreased positive emotional disregarding arousal levels. In comparison, paranoid schizophrenia has increased emotional responsivity to low arousing stimuli and reduced responsivity to high arousing stimuli. This study supports that Schizophrenia disturbs emotional experience.[3]

Traumatic brain injury is associated with reduced responsivity to negative affective stimuli. A study involving twenty-one TBI individuals uses picture stimuli to demonstrate that people TBI have normal emotional responsivity to pleasant pictures but show limited responses to unpleasant pictures. A potential explanation is that TBI damages the ventral surfaces of the frontal and temporal lobes, which are areas associated with emotional processing.[4]

Math anxiety describes the situation in which a person is overly distressed by math stimuli. A study involving fMRI techniques and 40 students demonstrates that people with math anxiety have increased emotional responsivity to math stimuli. The study suggests that when exposed to math-related stimuli, amygdala activity increases in participants' brain, which lowers the threshold of responding to a potential threat. Moreover, participants with math anxiety disengage and avoid the math stimuli more than images with negative valence such as a bleeding arm. The study suggests that math anxiety resembles other types of phobia in that there is increased vigilance and responsivity to specific stimuli.[10]

Post-traumatic stress disorder impacts emotional responsivity. A study showed that people with high PTSD severity abuse much more substances than others and have difficulties controlling their emotions. Not being able to regulate their emotions were found to have symptoms of PTSD and problems of alcohol and drug use.[13]

Emotional responsivity in substance-use

Cocaine-exposed infants

Using cocaine during pregnancy creates neurological damage to the fetus and neurobehavioral problems to the infants. 72 infants participated in a study where 36 infants have prenatal exposure to cocaine. The results demonstrate that compared to the control group, cocaine-exposed infants have decreased emotional responsivity, as they exhibit fewer expressions of joy, interest, surprise, anger, and sadness. Specifically, the cocaine-exposed group has reduced response to positive affective stimuli, suggesting that cocaine-exposure during pregnancy decreases feelings of joyfulness.[11]

Alcohol consumption impairs affective processing and therefore leads to abnormal responses to environmental stimuli. A study involving 42 abstinent alcoholics and 46 nonalcoholic demonstrates that alcoholics usually have lower emotional responsivity to erotic, happy, aversive, and gruesome stimuli. However, an in-depth analysis of fMRI images reveals gender differences. Alcoholic men have reduced emotional responsivity while alcoholic women have increased emotional responsivity to positive affective stimuli. The study suggests that the gender difference is associated with different functional abnormalities of emotional processing in the cortical, subcortical, and cerebellar regions of the brain.[5]

Opioids has shown to help decrease negative emotional responsivity. A study involving 21 people using opioids and 21 people not using opioids found that those using the drug had decreased levels of depression and elation compared to the other group. Those using the drug tend to show less emotion.[14]

Emotional responsivity in social factors

Social interaction

Emotional responsivity is said to have a unique association with social interaction. Studies suggest that social interaction, especially at home, can influence the way a child responds to emotional stimuli. For example, if child grew up in a home where emotional displays resulted in punishment or negative criticism, the child would have the tendency to find ways to hide their emotions.[15]

Sleeping issues in children have been linked to many physical and mental health problems later on in adulthood and created a greater risk for emotional and behavioral issues in children. Studies haven't been able to link the physiological functions with sleeping disturbances to these psychological consequences.[16] Emotional liability, responsivity, psychological responses to positive and negative picture stimuli have all been a result of sleep deprivation.

Doctors today are using neuroimaging to connect the relationship between sleep and neural mechanisms that cause emotional responsively in children. These studies resulted, "In general, the largest and most extensive sleep-related correlations for any emotion were found for disgust expressions".[16]

Treatment

Emotion Regulation Therapy (ERT)

A developed treatment that combines principles from traditional and contemporary cognitive therapy. ERT looks to understand and help individuals with mental illnesses. Experiments have shown support for the use of ERT as it has shown better results than already established treatments.[17]

References

  1. ^ Mathews, J.R.; Barch, D.M. (2010). "Emotion responsivity, social cognition, and functional outcome in schizophrenia". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 119 (1): 50–59. doi:10.1037/a0017861. PMC 2849101. PMID 20141242.
  2. ^ a b Carels, R. A.; Blumenthal, J. A.; Sherwood, A. (April 2000). "Emotional responsivity during daily life: relationship to psychosocial functioning and ambulatory blood pressure". International Journal of Psychophysiology. 36 (1): 25–33. doi:10.1016/s0167-8760(99)00101-4. ISSN 0167-8760. PMID 10700620.
  3. ^ a b c d Lee, Eun; Kim, Jae-Jin; Namkoong, Kee; An, Suk Kyoon; Seok, Jeong-Ho; Lee, Yu Jin; Kang, Jee In; Choi, Jae Hyuk; Hong, Taekyong; Jeon, Jong Hee; Lee, Hong Shick (2006). "Aberrantly flattened responsivity to emotional pictures in paranoid schizophrenia". Psychiatry Research. 143 (2–3): 135–145. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.011. PMID 16884781. S2CID 24230799.
  4. ^ a b c d de Sousa, Arielle; McDonald, Skye; Rushby, Jacqueline; Li, Sophie; Dimoska, Aneta; James, Charlotte (2010). "Why don't you feel how I feel? Insight into the absence of empathy after severe Traumatic Brain Injury". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3585–3595. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.008. PMID 20713073. S2CID 25275909.
  5. ^ a b c Sawyer, Kayle S; Maleki, Nasim; Urban, Trinity; Marinkovic, Ksenija; Karson, Steven; Ruiz, Susan M; Harris, Gordon J; Oscar-Berman, Marlene (2019-04-30). "Alcoholism gender differences in brain responsivity to emotional stimuli". eLife. 8: e41723. doi:10.7554/eLife.41723. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 6491039. PMID 31038125.
  6. ^ a b c Weiss, Elisabeth M.; Rominger, Christian; Hofer, Ellen; Fink, Andreas; Papousek, Ilona (2019). "Less differentiated facial responses to naturalistic films of another person's emotional expressions in adolescents and adults with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 89: 341–346. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.10.007. PMID 30336172. S2CID 53013060.
  7. ^ Sigman, M.D.; Kasari, C.; Kwon, J.; Yirmiya, N. (1992). "Responses to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children". Child Development. 63 (4): 796–807. doi:10.2307/1131234. JSTOR 1131234. PMID 1505241.
  8. ^ Repacholi, B. M.; Gopnick, A. (1997). "Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds". Developmental Psychology. 33 (1): 12–21. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12. PMID 9050386.
  9. ^ a b Scambler, D. J.; Hepburn, S.; Rutherford, M. D.; Wehner, E. A.; Rogers, S. J. (2007-03-01). "Emotional Responsivity in Children with Autism, Children with Other Developmental Disabilities, and Children with Typical Development". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 37 (3): 553–563. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0186-y. ISSN 0162-3257. PMID 16933089. S2CID 2213430.
  10. ^ a b c Pizzie, Rachel G.; Kraemer, David J.M. (2017). "Avoiding math on a rapid timescale: Emotional responsivity and anxious attention in math anxiety". Brain and Cognition. 118: 100–107. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2017.08.004. PMID 28826050. S2CID 8013856.
  11. ^ a b Alessandri, Steven M.; Sullivan, Margaret W.; Imaizumi, Sonia; Lewis, Michael (1993). "Learning and emotional responsivity in cocaine-exposed infants". Developmental Psychology. 29 (6): 989–997. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.989. ISSN 0012-1649.
  12. ^ Leung, Winnie W.; Couture, Shannon M.; Blanchard, Jack J.; Lin, Stephanie; Llerena, Katiah (2010). "Is social anhedonia related to emotional responsivity and expressivity? A laboratory study in women". Schizophrenia Research. 124 (1–3): 66–73. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.012. PMC 2962705. PMID 20620020.
  13. ^ Weiss, Nicole H.; Schick, Melissa R.; Contractor, Ateka A.; Dixon-Gordon, Katherine L. (2019-09-01). "Posttraumatic stress disorder and substance use: Identifying the underlying role of difficulties regulating positive emotions". Addictive Behaviors. 96: 119–126. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.04.029. ISSN 0306-4603. PMID 31075729. S2CID 149454349.
  14. ^ Savvas, Steven M.; Somogyi, Andrew A.; White, Jason M. (February 2012). "The effect of methadone on emotional reactivity". Addiction. 107 (2): 388–392. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2011.03634.x. ISSN 1360-0443. PMID 21883603.
  15. ^ Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Carlo, G., & Karbon, M. (1992). "Emotional responsivity to others: Behavioral correlates and socialization antecedents". New Directions for Child Development. 1992 (55): 57–73. doi:10.1002/cd.23219925506. PMID 1608515.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ a b Reidy, Hamann, Inman, Johnson, Brennan, Brooke L., Stephan, Cory, Katrina C., Patricia A. (January 24, 2016). "Decreased sleep duration is associated with increased fMRI responses to emotional faces in children". Neuropsychologia. 84: 54–62. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.01.028. PMID 26821063. S2CID 25259283.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ Renna, Megan; Quintero, Jean; Fresco, David; Mennin, Douglas (2017). "Emotion Regulation Therapy: A mechanism-targeted treatment for disorders of distress". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 98. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00098. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5292405. PMID 28220089.

Read other articles:

Ця стаття потребує додаткових посилань на джерела для поліпшення її перевірності. Будь ласка, допоможіть удосконалити цю статтю, додавши посилання на надійні (авторитетні) джерела. Зверніться на сторінку обговорення за поясненнями та допоможіть виправити недоліки. Матер…

Si ce bandeau n'est plus pertinent, retirez-le. Cliquez ici pour en savoir plus. Cet article ne s'appuie pas, ou pas assez, sur des sources secondaires ou tertiaires (novembre 2023). Pour améliorer la vérifiabilité de l'article ainsi que son intérêt encyclopédique, il est nécessaire, quand des sources primaires sont citées, de les associer à des analyses faites par des sources secondaires. Cet article est une ébauche concernant une compétition cycliste. Vous pouvez partager vos connai…

Pedestrian mall and attraction in Madison, Wisconsin, US State StreetState St. from Library mall, October 2007Length0.78 mi (1.26 km)LocationMadison, Wisconsin, USConstructionConstruction startJune 1974 State Street is a pedestrian zone located in downtown Madison, Wisconsin, United States, near the State Capitol. The road proper extends from the west corner of land comprising the Capitol (on the southwestern edge of the Madison Isthmus, at the corners of Carroll and Mifflin Streets) w…

يفتقر محتوى هذه المقالة إلى الاستشهاد بمصادر. فضلاً، ساهم في تطوير هذه المقالة من خلال إضافة مصادر موثوق بها. أي معلومات غير موثقة يمكن التشكيك بها وإزالتها. (ديسمبر 2018)Learn how and when to remove this message 139° خط طول 139 غرب خريطة لجميع الإحداثيات من جوجل خريطة لجميع الإحداثيات من بينغ تصدير…

1941 prototype fighter aircraft XP-46 Curtiss XP-46 Role FighterType of aircraft Manufacturer Curtiss-Wright Corporation First flight 15 February 1941 Primary user United States Army Air Corps (intended) Number built 2 Developed from Curtiss P-40 Developed into Curtiss XP-53 The Curtiss XP-46 was a 1940s American prototype fighter aircraft. It was a development of the Curtiss-Wright Corporation in an effort to introduce the best features found in European fighter aircraft in 1939 into a fig…

Cet article est une ébauche concernant la microbiologie. Vous pouvez partager vos connaissances en l’améliorant (comment ?) selon les recommandations des projets correspondants. Si ce bandeau n'est plus pertinent, retirez-le. Cliquez ici pour en savoir plus. Cet article ne cite pas suffisamment ses sources (novembre 2022). Si vous disposez d'ouvrages ou d'articles de référence ou si vous connaissez des sites web de qualité traitant du thème abordé ici, merci de compléter l'article…

President of Poland from 2005 to 2010 Lech KaczyńskiOfficial portrait, 2006President of PolandIn office23 December 2005 – 10 April 2010Prime MinisterKazimierz MarcinkiewiczJarosław KaczyńskiDonald TuskPreceded byAleksander KwaśniewskiSucceeded byBronisław Komorowski (acting)President of the Supreme Audit OfficeIn office14 February 1992 – 8 June 1995PresidentLech WałęsaPrime MinisterJan OlszewskiWaldemar PawlakHanna SuchockaWaldemar PawlakJózef OleksyPreceded byWaler…

Gerhard Lagerstråle Född25 februari 1814[1][2]Karlskrona amiralitetsförsamling[1][2], SverigeDöd18 april 1887[1][2] (73 år)Nättraby församling[1][2], SverigeMedborgare iSverigeUtbildad vidUppsala universitet[3][2] SysselsättningPolitiker[2], jurist, domareBefattningLedamot av Sveriges ståndsriksdag (1844–1845)[2]Ledamot av Sveriges ståndsriksdag (1850–1866)[2]Justitieombudsmannen (1857–1858)Konsultativt statsrådRegeringen De Geer d.ä. I (1858–1860)[2]Sveriges c…

Clustering of stars in astronomy diagram The red clump is the prominent group of red giant stars at about 5,000 K and 75 L☉. The red clump is a clustering of red giants in the Hertzsprung–Russell diagram at around 5,000 K and absolute magnitude (MV) +0.5, slightly hotter than most red-giant-branch stars of the same luminosity. It is visible as a denser region of the red-giant branch or a bulge towards hotter temperatures. It is prominent in many galactic open clusters, and it is al…

Cette page concerne l'année 1751 du calendrier grégorien. Chronologies Fort Beausejour en 1755.Données clés 1748 1749 1750  1751  1752 1753 1754Décennies :1720 1730 1740  1750  1760 1770 1780Siècles :XVIe XVIIe  XVIIIe  XIXe XXeMillénaires :-Ier Ier  IIe  IIIe Chronologies thématiques Art Architecture, Arts plastiques (Dessin, Gravure, Peinture et Sculpture), (), Littérature (), Musique (Classique) et Théâtre   Ingénierie ()…

Ukrainian writer, poet and artist (1814–1861) Taras ShevchenkoТарас ШевченкоPortrait of Taras Shevchenko, by Ivan Kramskoi in 1871Born9 March 1814Moryntsi, Kiev Governorate, Russian EmpireDied10 March 1861 (aged 47)Saint Petersburg, Russian EmpirePen nameKobzarOccupation Poet philosopher painter graphic artist ethnographer activist LanguageUkrainian, RussianNationalityUkrainianCitizenshipRussian EmpireAlma materImperial Academy of ArtsPeriod1832–1861Notable worksKobzarRelat…

Hockey draft 2023 PWHL DraftGeneral informationDate(s)September 18, 2023LocationCBC's Toronto HeadquartersToronto, Ontario, CanadaNetwork(s)CBC GemCBC Sports AppOverview90 total selections in 15 roundsLeagueProfessional Women's Hockey LeagueFirst selection Taylor HeiseMinnesota2024 → The 2023 PWHL draft was the first draft in Professional Women's Hockey League history, and took place on September 18, 2023, at the Canadian Broadcasting Centre in Toronto, Ontario.[1] Minnes…

Universitas BresciaUniversità degli Studi di BresciaJenisNegeriDidirikan1982RektorProf. Maurizio TiraStaf administrasi500Jumlah mahasiswa14.000LokasiBrescia, ItaliaTim olahragaCUS Brescia (http://www.cusbrescia.it/)Situs webwww.unibs.it/ Universitas Brescia (bahasa Italia: Università degli Studi di Brescia) adalah sebuah universitas penelitian negeri di Brescia, Italia. Universitas ini didirikan pada tahun 1982 dan dibagi ke dalam 4 sekolah. Universitas Brescia resmi didirikan pada tahun 1…

Peta infrastruktur dan tata guna lahan di Komune Houéville.  = Kawasan perkotaan  = Lahan subur  = Padang rumput  = Lahan pertanaman campuran  = Hutan  = Vegetasi perdu  = Lahan basah  = Anak sungaiHouéville merupakan sebuah komune di departemen Vosges yang terletak pada sebelah timur laut Prancis. Lihat pula Komune di departemen Vosges Referensi INSEE lbsKomune di departemen Vosges Les Ableuvenettes Ahéville Aingeville Ainvelle Allarmont Ambacourt Ameu…

Anthony Modeste Informasi pribadiNama lengkap Anthony ModesteTanggal lahir 14 April 1988 (umur 36)Tempat lahir Cannes, PrancisTinggi 1,86 m (6 ft 1 in)[1]Posisi bermain PenyerangInformasi klubKlub saat ini 1899 HoffenheimNomor 27Karier junior2001–2003 Fréjus2003–2007 NiceKarier senior*Tahun Tim Tampil (Gol)2007–2010 Nice 42 (3)2009–2010 → Angers (pinjaman) 37 (20)2010–2013 Bordeaux 52 (13)2012 → Blackburn Rovers (pinjaman) 5 (0)2012–2013 → Bastia (p…

У этого термина существуют и другие значения, см. Свод (значения). Готические своды Сен-Шапель Свод (от «сводить» — соединять, смыкать) — в архитектуре тип перекрытия или покрытия пространства (помещения), ограниченного стенами, балками или столбами — конструкция, кот…

Matisse Thybulle Matisse Thybulle en 2019. Fiche d’identité Nom complet Matisse Vincent Thybulle Nationalité États-Unis Australie Naissance 4 mars 1997 (27 ans)Scottsdale, Arizona Taille 1,96 m (6′ 5″) Poids 91 kg (200 lb) Situation en club Club actuel Trail Blazers de Portland Numéro 4 Poste Arrière / Ailier Carrière universitaire ou amateur 2015-2019 Huskies de Washington Draft de la NBA Année 2019 Position 20e Franchise Celtics de Boston Carrière profess…

Major character of the epic Mahabharata For other uses, see Vidura (disambiguation). Fictional character ViduraPersonal InformationAffiliationKuru dynastyWeaponSwordFamilyParents see Niyoga Vyasa (father)Parishrami (mother) Half-brothers Dhritarashtra by AmbikaPandu by AmbalikaSpouse[[]]ChildrenYuyutsu Unknown twins daughtersRelativesHalf-brother through Vyasa see NiyogaShuka (from Vatikā) This article contains Indic text. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks or boxes, m…

You can help expand this article with text translated from the corresponding article in French. (December 2008) Click [show] for important translation instructions. View a machine-translated version of the French article. Machine translation, like DeepL or Google Translate, is a useful starting point for translations, but translators must revise errors as necessary and confirm that the translation is accurate, rather than simply copy-pasting machine-translated text into the English Wikipedi…

  لمعانٍ أخرى، طالع العين (توضيح). العين تقسيم إداري البلد  السعودية التقسيم الأعلى منطقة الرياض  السكان التعداد السكاني غير معروف نسمة (إحصاء ) تعديل مصدري - تعديل   العين، هي قرية من فئة (ب) تقع في محافظة الدلم، والتابعة لمنطقة الرياض في السعودية. وتبعد العين عن مح…